This study investigated the diurnal variation in self-reported mental health and well-being (MHW) using data from the UCL COVID-19 Social Study, a large longitudinal panel study. The analysis included 49,218 adults in England, with an average of 18.5 observations per person. Linear mixed-effects models were used to analyze the data, controlling for various demographic, socioeconomic, and health-related covariates. The key finding was a nonlinear relationship between time of day and MHW, with individuals generally reporting better MHW in the morning (lowest depressive and anxiety symptoms, highest happiness, life satisfaction, and sense of life being worthwhile) and worse MHW around midnight. The difference between early morning and midnight could be as high as 10% of the standard deviation for depressive and anxiety symptoms, and up to 15% for well-being measures. There was also evidence of associations with day of the week (with more variation during weekends) and season (with better MHW in the summer). The association between time of day and MHW was moderated by day of the week, with different patterns observed for depressive and anxiety symptoms on Tuesdays and Wednesdays compared to other days.
The study provides strong evidence for diurnal variation in self-reported mental health and well-being (MHW), showing a consistent pattern of better MHW in the morning and worse MHW around midnight. It also demonstrates associations with day of the week and season, and a moderation effect of day of the week. However, it is crucial to note that these findings demonstrate correlation, not causation. The study design does not allow for conclusions about whether time of day *causes* changes in MHW, or vice-versa.
The study's large sample size and longitudinal design are major strengths, allowing for the examination of within-person variations. The use of standardized measures for depression and anxiety, and the inclusion of multiple aspects of well-being, enhance the study's validity. The weighting procedure increases the generalizability of the findings to the English population. The practical implications are substantial, suggesting that time of day, day of week, and season should be considered in research design, intervention delivery, and public health service provision. For example, mental health services might need to adjust staffing levels or resource allocation based on expected diurnal and seasonal variations in demand.
Despite these strengths, the study acknowledges key uncertainties. The non-random nature of the sample and potential biases in weighting limit the generalizability beyond the English population. The study also highlights the possibility of reverse causality, meaning that individuals' MHW might influence *when* they choose to complete the survey, rather than time of day directly influencing MHW. Future research should use methods like ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to address this limitation and explore directionality. The study provides clear guidance for researchers and practitioners, emphasizing the importance of accounting for temporal factors, but also acknowledges the need for further investigation.
Critical unanswered questions remain. What are the specific mechanisms underlying the observed diurnal variations? Are there other moderators, such as latitude, climate, or cultural factors, that influence these patterns? How do individual differences in chronotype (e.g., morningness-eveningness) interact with time of day to affect MHW? While the methodological limitations, particularly the potential for reverse causality, do not fundamentally invalidate the conclusions about *associations*, they do significantly affect the interpretation of *causal* relationships. Further research is needed to disentangle the complex interplay of factors influencing MHW across different time scales.
The abstract clearly states the research question, investigating the diurnal variation in self-reported mental health and well-being, a relatively understudied area.
The abstract concisely summarizes the methodology, mentioning the large sample size, data source, and statistical approach.
The abstract presents the main findings, highlighting the diurnal pattern, day-of-week and seasonal associations, and the moderation effect of weekends.
The abstract succinctly states the overall conclusion and its implications for research and clinical practice.
This is a medium-impact improvement as it would improve clarity and provide important context for the reader. The Abstract is the first point of contact with the research and needs to give the reader a clear idea of the study population. This is important for understanding the generalizability of the findings.
Implementation: Specify the country/region of the study participants. For example, 'The study analysed data from 49,218 adults in [Country/Region]...'
This is a high-impact improvement. While the abstract mentions 'linear mixed-effects models,' specifying the outcome variables would significantly enhance clarity and informativeness. This is crucial for readers to quickly grasp the scope of the study. The Abstract should be self-contained and not require the reader to go to the main text to understand what was measured.
Implementation: Explicitly list the key outcome variables. For example, after '...linear mixed-effects models', add: '...to assess the impact of time of day, day of the week, and season on depression, anxiety, well-being (happiness, life satisfaction, and sense of life being worthwhile), and loneliness.'
This is a low-impact improvement. While the abstract mentions 'moderated by day,' specifying *how* it is moderated (i.e., greater variation on weekends) adds a layer of detail that enhances understanding without significantly increasing length. This belongs in the Abstract as it is a key finding.
Implementation: Replace 'Time-of-day patterns are moderated by day, with more variation in mental health and individual well-being during weekends compared with weekdays.' with a slightly more detailed phrase, such as: 'Time-of-day patterns were moderated by day of the week, showing greater variation in mental health and well-being during weekends compared to weekdays.'
The introduction effectively establishes the dynamic nature of mental health and well-being (MHW), highlighting its fluctuations across various time scales, from life course changes to monthly variations during the COVID-19 pandemic.
The introduction clearly identifies a gap in the existing literature: the lack of research on diurnal (daily) changes in MHW, contrasting it with the more extensive research on mood fluctuations.
The introduction provides a strong theoretical rationale for expecting diurnal changes in MHW, drawing from physiological processes (neurotransmitters, hormones, inflammatory markers), geographical and environmental factors, and daily activity patterns.
The introduction concisely reviews the existing, albeit limited and mixed, evidence on diurnal variations in mood, highlighting findings from social media data analyses and studies on specific emotions.
The introduction effectively connects diurnal variations to longer-term (weekly and seasonal) changes in mood and MHW, citing research on seasonal affective disorder and Google search trends.
The introduction clearly states the study's aim: to examine the association between time of day and MHW, including various components (depressive symptoms, anxiety, hedonic well-being, eudemonic well-being, and social well-being), and to explore variations across days, seasons, and years.
The introduction concisely highlights the implications of the research questions for study design, analysis, intervention delivery, and public health services.
This is a medium-impact improvement. While the introduction mentions the influence of geographical and environmental factors, it could benefit from briefly elaborating on *how* these factors might contribute to diurnal variations in MHW. The Introduction section's role is to provide context and rationale, and this expansion strengthens that rationale. This would provide a more complete picture of the potential mechanisms underlying the hypothesized diurnal changes, enhancing the reader's understanding of the research context.
Implementation: Expand the discussion of geographical and environmental factors. For instance, after '...some of which may vary with systematic patterns across the day,' add a sentence like: 'For example, variations in sunlight exposure throughout the day can influence melatonin production, which in turn can affect mood and sleep patterns, potentially contributing to diurnal changes in MHW.'
This is a low-impact improvement. The introduction could be slightly strengthened by more explicitly stating the novelty of the study. While the research gap is identified, the introduction could more forcefully state *why* this study is unique and important in addressing that gap. The Introduction should set the stage for the study's contribution, and emphasizing novelty helps achieve this. This would further emphasize the study's contribution to the field and its potential impact.
Implementation: Add a sentence or phrase emphasizing the study's novelty. For example, before the final paragraph, add a sentence like: 'To our knowledge, this is the first large-scale study to comprehensively examine diurnal patterns across a wide range of MHW measures, while also considering the influence of day of the week, season, and year.'
This is a low-impact improvement that enhances the clarity and flow of the introduction. The transition between discussing mood and broader MHW could be made smoother. The Introduction should build a logical argument, and this refinement improves that flow. This would improve the logical connection between the existing literature on mood and the current study's focus on broader MHW.
Implementation: Add a transitional phrase or sentence. For example, after '...different patterns across the day.13 14', add: 'While these studies provide valuable insights into mood fluctuations, they do not fully address the broader construct of mental health and well-being, which encompasses a wider range of psychological and social factors.'
The Methods section clearly describes the data source, the UCL COVID-19 Social Study, including its longitudinal design, sample size, and recruitment methods. This provides essential context for understanding the study's scope and limitations.
The section specifies the outcome measures, including standardized instruments for depression (PHQ-9) and anxiety (GAD-7), and adapted items for well-being and loneliness. This allows for a clear understanding of the constructs being assessed.
The section clearly defines the exposure variables, including time of day, day of the week, astronomical season, and survey year, all derived from timestamps. This provides a precise operationalization of the temporal factors.
The section lists the covariates measured at baseline, including demographic, socioeconomic, and health-related factors. This indicates that the analysis accounted for potential confounding variables.
The section specifies the statistical analysis method, linear mixed-effects models, which is appropriate for analyzing longitudinal data with repeated measures. It also mentions testing for nonlinear trajectories and interactions.
The section describes the weighting procedure used to account for the non-random nature of the sample, enhancing the generalizability of the findings to the English population.
This is a medium-impact improvement that would enhance the study's methodological transparency and reproducibility. The Methods section is crucial for allowing other researchers to fully understand and potentially replicate the study. While the section mentions the use of linear mixed-effects models, it doesn't specify the random effects structure (e.g., random intercepts for participants, random slopes for time). This detail is crucial for understanding how the model accounts for individual differences and the correlation of repeated measures within individuals. Providing this information would strengthen the paper by enabling other researchers to replicate the analysis and build upon this work. It also enhances the transparency of the statistical methods.
Implementation: Specify the random effects structure of the linear mixed-effects models. For example, state: 'Models included random intercepts for participants and random slopes for time (or whichever structure was used) to account for individual differences and the correlation of repeated measures within individuals.' This could be added after the sentence describing the use of linear mixed-effects models.
This is a medium-impact improvement. The Methods section should provide sufficient detail for other researchers to understand and replicate the study. While the section mentions excluding participants with missing data, it doesn't state the *amount* of missing data for each variable. This is important for assessing the potential for bias due to missing data. Including this detail would strengthen the paper by providing a more complete picture of the data quality and potential limitations. This would also allow readers to assess the potential impact of missing data on the study's findings.
Implementation: Report the amount of missing data for each variable, either in the main text or in a supplementary table. For example, state: 'X% of participants had missing data on [variable 1], Y% on [variable 2], etc.' This could be added after the sentence describing the exclusion of participants with missing data.
This is a low-impact improvement. While the Methods section describes the survey invitation and reminder process, it could be more explicit about the *potential* for selection bias introduced by this process. The Methods section should acknowledge potential limitations of the study design. Participants who respond to email invitations and reminders might differ systematically from those who do not, potentially affecting the generalizability of the findings. Acknowledging this potential bias would strengthen the paper by demonstrating a critical awareness of the study's limitations. This would also encourage readers to interpret the findings in light of this potential bias.
Implementation: Add a sentence acknowledging the potential for selection bias. For example: 'The reliance on email invitations and reminders may have introduced selection bias, as participants who are more responsive to such prompts may differ systematically from those who are not.' This could be added at the end of the paragraph describing the survey invitation process.
The Results section clearly presents the demographic characteristics of the sample, both before and after weighting, allowing readers to assess the representativeness of the data.
The section reports the within-individual and between-individual variance for the outcome measures, providing valuable information about the stability and variability of these constructs.
The section presents the main findings on the association between time of day and MHW, indicating a nonlinear relationship and highlighting the general pattern of better outcomes in the morning and worse outcomes at night.
The section reports the findings on the association between day of the week and MHW, noting some inconsistencies but also highlighting specific differences, such as higher depressive symptoms on Wednesdays and Thursdays compared to Sunday.
The section clearly presents the findings on the seasonal effects, showing consistent evidence for better MHW in the summer across all outcomes.
The section reports the findings on the moderation analysis, indicating that only the interactions between time and day of the week were statistically significant and describing the different patterns observed for specific days.
The section uses figures (Figures 1, 2, and 3) effectively to visually represent the findings, enhancing the clarity and accessibility of the results.
This is a medium-impact improvement. While the Results section presents the findings, it could be strengthened by providing effect sizes (e.g., standardized mean differences or other relevant metrics) for the observed associations. The Results section's purpose is to present the findings in a clear and objective manner, and including effect sizes enhances the interpretability of the results. Reporting effect sizes would allow readers to better understand the *magnitude* of the observed effects, not just their statistical significance. This is important for assessing the practical significance of the findings and for comparing the results with other studies. It would enhance the paper by providing a more complete and informative picture of the findings, facilitating their interpretation and comparison with other research.
Implementation: Include effect sizes (e.g., standardized mean differences, Cohen's d, or other appropriate metrics) alongside the p-values and confidence intervals when reporting the results of the statistical analyses. This could be done in the text or in tables/figures. For example, when reporting the difference between morning and midnight MHW, state: 'The difference between early morning and midnight could be as high as 10% of the SD for depressive and anxiety symptoms' and also provide a standardized effect size.
This is a medium-impact improvement. The Results section should present the findings in a clear and objective manner, and explicitly stating the statistical significance (p-values) for all reported associations enhances the rigor and transparency of the results. While the section mentions that some associations were statistically significant, it doesn't consistently report p-values for all findings. Providing p-values for all reported associations (e.g., day of the week, season, year) would allow readers to directly assess the statistical significance of each finding. This is important for evaluating the strength of the evidence and for comparing the results with other studies. This would enhance the paper by providing a more complete and transparent presentation of the statistical results, allowing readers to fully evaluate the evidence.
Implementation: Report the p-values for all reported associations, either in the text or in the figures/tables. For example, when reporting the association between day of the week and MHW, state the specific p-values for each comparison (e.g., 'depressive symptoms were higher on Wednesdays (p = 0.02) and Thursdays (p = 0.03) compared with Sunday').
This is a low-impact improvement. The Results section should present the findings in a way that is easy to understand and interpret. While the section mentions 'worthwhile ratings' multiple times, it could be clearer and more consistent to use the full term 'sense of life being worthwhile' or a consistent abbreviation (e.g., 'worthwhile ratings (sense of life being worthwhile)') throughout. The current phrasing might be slightly ambiguous for some readers. Using consistent terminology would improve the clarity and readability of the Results section. This is a minor improvement, but it would enhance the overall professionalism and clarity of the presentation.
Implementation: Use consistent terminology for 'worthwhile ratings'. Either use the full term 'sense of life being worthwhile' throughout, or introduce a consistent abbreviation (e.g., 'worthwhile ratings (sense of life being worthwhile)') and use it consistently. For example, change 'Worthwhile ratings had the most variation...' to 'Worthwhile ratings (sense of life being worthwhile) had the most variation...'
Figure 1 Predicted changes over time from linear mixed-effects model for each outcome (covariates fixed at reference values): (a) depressive symptoms, (b) anxiety symptoms, (c) happiness, (d) life satisfaction, (e) worthwhile and (f) loneliness. std, standardised.
Figure 2 Coefficients and 95% confidence intervals from linear mixed-effects model showing average differences by day, season and year for each outcome: (a) depressive symptoms, (b) anxiety symptoms, (c) happiness, (d) life satisfaction, (e) worthwhile and (f) loneliness. Reference categories were Sunday (day), winter (season) and 2020 (year).
Figure 3 Predicted changes over time from linear mixed-effects model with moderation by day of the week for each outcome (covariates fixed at reference values): (a) depressive symptoms, (b) anxiety symptoms, (c) happiness, (d) life satisfaction, (e) worthwhile and (f) loneliness. Solid lines representing significantly different patterns to Sunday (p<0.05). std, standardised.